4.4 The Evolution of the Visual System
К оглавлению1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 1617 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33
34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41
So, how did this wiring come into existence? And how, ultimately, did it
form into the complex wiring that comprises the human visual system,
eventually enabling humans to solve vision-related, nonroutine problems
creatively with the use of scenario visualization?
As noted earlier, organisms fi rst developed a light/dark sensitivity area.
We have already spoken about the euglena’s eyespot, but hydras and
worms also have light/dark sensitivity cells located in their outer epidermal
layers. Eventually, a pinhole eyespot formed, which enabled even more
complex processing of visual information. Of course, in order for this
eyespot to have occurred, other parts of a rudimentary nervous system had
to be in place, like the mass of neurons localized in one area, such as the
head.
For example, the nautilus—one of the so-called living fossils, because it
morphologically has not changed much since the Cambrian explosion—is
representative of this next step in the evolution of the eye. The evolution
of the visual system likely began with a general light sensitivity around the ventricle of the forebrain. These forebrain neurons were transformed
in the course of evolution into an assemblage of neurons capable of analyzing
strictly visual information, rather than other information carried to
this area by other input neurons. The processed visual information was
transferred through relay neurons (the future ganglion cells) to the contralateral
part of the brain. In subsequent generations, this primordial
retina evaginated (formed a cup-like hole) and eventually transformed into
the lateral eyes of vertebrates (Cronly-Dillon, 1991).
Once this rudimentary eye developed, it took different directions in
terms of its formation among the various species that have vision. There
are all types of eyes falling into two broad categories: the compound eyes
of insects, crabs, and arthropods that are composed of hundreds of cylindrical
elements consisting of a lens, eight photoreceptor cells, and a sleeve
of opaque pigment cells, and the vertebrate eyes of fi sh, reptiles, and
mammals that all build upon the basic fl atworm eye having a retina, ganglion
cells, and projections to a brain. This divergence further ratifi es the
evolutionary principles of genetic variability and the natural selection of
the fi ttest traits given a specifi c environment. The kind of eye that fortuitously
developed, and then fl ourished, in the sand shark population under
the sea will be different from the eye that fortuitously developed, and then
fl ourished, in the black widow spider population on land, and so on.
As noted already, through investigation of the fossil endocasts of insectivores,
Jerison (1973, 1991) has offered a convincing case that the ancestor
of the primate was a small, nocturnal creature that prowled through trees
and bushes on the lookout for insects, as some small primates do today.
The eyes of this creature were close set and forward facing, indicating that
it was a predator. Predators like owls, cats, and primates have close set,
forward facing eyes with stereotopic vision, enabling them to more easily
identify depths and distances while hunting their prey. Animals like rabbits
and squirrels—the prey of predators like owls, cats, and some primates—
have eyes on the sides of their heads with panoramic vision, enabling them
to more easily identify predators from a variety of angles. The eyes of this
insectivore would have had an abundance of rods and fewer cones to
enable maneuverability in the dark. Insectivores had to forage on the
ground and in the dark in order to steer clear of bigger reptiles, amphibians,
and dinosaurs that would have preyed on them during that time.
However, when most of the dinosaurs and other larger predators were
wiped out by an asteroid at the close of the Cretaceous, this allowed for
the possibility of insectivores to occupy different niches. Eventually, some
of the descendents of these creatures did their foraging during the day,
The Evolution of the Visual System and Scenario Visualization 107
while others took to life in the trees. Still others, like our primate ancestors,
did both. By 20 mya, Proconsul had taken to life in the trees of Africa, most
likely because such a life off of the ground made it easier to avoid predators
lurking on the ground, as well as pounce upon prey that happened to be
on the ground. This life in the trees is evidenced by their money-like and
ape-like skeletons as well as their diet, which indicate a lifestyle not unlike
that of present-day monkeys and apes (Groves, 2002; Hartwig, 2002;
McGrew, 2004; Byrne & Whiten, 1988; Allman, 1982, 2000).
Life during the daytime and in the trees meant that a visual system, at
the very least, would need to have evolved more cones, the capacity to
distinguish colors, more memory-storage capacities, and facial recognition.
More cones would be needed to be able to handle the brighter light of
daytime living. A capacity to distinguish colors would be needed to determine
what foods one could eat, as well as distinguish foreground from
background while maneuvering through the trees. Rudimentary forms of
long-term and short-term memory would be needed for a mental map of
the area where this creature lived, as well as for distinguishing friend from
foe. Finally, facial recognition would be needed to distinguish friend from
foe, as well as to communicate various messages like “Leave me alone,”
“Back off,” “It’s o.k. to touch me,” and the like to other members of one’s
social group or to enemies. On top of all of this, arboreal life required
extremities more fi t for grasping, jumping, launching, and other sorts of
bodily maneuvering (Barton, 1998; Fleagle, 1999; Byrne, 1995, 2001; Rilling
& Insel, 1998; Allman, 1977).
In order to do all of this, the connections between eye and brain, as well
as those connections between other sensorimotor systems and brain, would
need to be more complex. As Jerison (1973, 1997) and Kaas (1996) point
out, the visual system of this early arboreal ancestor would have had a
brain very similar to that of a lemur or a monkey. This being the case, at
least three features of the brain and visual system would have needed to
be in place in Proconsul.
First, referring back to MacLean’s model, the reptilian, the paleomammalian,
and part of the neomammalian brain would have to be present in
Proconsul, since the amount of processing that such arboreal ancestors had
to do required a brain larger than that of reptiles. The amount of processing
that the visual system does alone would constitute a bigger brain.
However, arboreal social life entailed the interaction of sensory modalities
in combination with memory, emotion, planning, and motor function. All
of this processing required a bigger brain, more synaptic connections, and
more types of neural connections. In addition, the system must have been
a hierarchical organization of processes and subsystems so that the right
data could be selected and information could be integrated as Proconsul
went about the business of negotiating its environment.
Second, in order to organize and coordinate information from the specialized
visual modules and each of the other sensory modalities, integrative
or association areas and systems had to evolve. Research by Kaas (1987,
1993, 1995, 1996), Bear et al. (2001), Uylings & van Eden (1990), Roberts,
Robbins, & Weiskrantz (1998), and Hofman (2001) has shown that it is
primarily the association areas of the brain that increase in the evolution
of the mammal. The association areas show a marked increase when we
compare the brains of rats, cats, macaque monkeys, and humans, in that
order. On inspection, the visual, auditory, and sensorimotor areas of a rat
basically butt up against one another with very little of the cortex—some
15%–20%—devoted to the association of these areas. Some 40%–50% of
the frontal, parietal, and temporal lobes of the macaque monkey are
devoted to association of sensorimotor information. Even more cortex—
some 50%–60%—is devoted to association of sensorimotor information in
humans.
Third, the visual areas would have needed to be linked up with the motor
areas, so that visual information could be communicated to the brain and
then translated into voluntary motion. The motor areas of primates receive
inputs from the thalamic nuclei that relay information from the basal telencephalon
and the cerebellum, and they send outputs to motor control
neurons in the brain and spinal cord. The motor area is situated next to
the somatosensory area, and both are located in the frontal lobe near the
central sulcus at the parietofrontal junction of the brain. The sensorimotor
area—along with the auditory and visual areas—send projections to association
areas in the parietal and temporal cortices. From there, the projections
are sent to the multimodal areas located in the parietotemporal,
prefrontal, and limbic cortices (Kandel et al., 2000). These connections
would have had to be in place in the brain of Proconsul so that it could
have acted on visual information it received.
In the last chapter, I mentioned the idea put forward by the Gestalt
theorists and William James that the mind and the world are “something
of a mutual fi t.” This idea should be kept in mind here as we investigate
the evolution of visual modules, along with the evolution of other sensory
modules and mechanisms of selectivity and integration. Events in the
physical environment are composed of materials that the human brain,
complete with its specialized modules, has evolved to perceive and discriminate.
These materials take forms ranging from specifi c chemicals, to
The Evolution of the Visual System and Scenario Visualization 109
mechanical energy, to electromagnetic radiation (light) and are discriminated
by the different sensory modalities that are specifi cally attuned to
these stimuli: taste and smell are specialized for processing concentrations
of different chemicals, touch is specialized for processing mechanical
deformations of the skin, hearing is specialized for the intake of sound
waves, and vision is specialized for the intake of electromagnetic radiation.
Thus, given the environment surrounding an animal, it makes sense that
the CNS is a hierarchically organized system composed of specialized
modules, as well as mechanisms of selectivity and integration, in order to
process the myriad pieces of environmental information.
So, how did this wiring come into existence? And how, ultimately, did it
form into the complex wiring that comprises the human visual system,
eventually enabling humans to solve vision-related, nonroutine problems
creatively with the use of scenario visualization?
As noted earlier, organisms fi rst developed a light/dark sensitivity area.
We have already spoken about the euglena’s eyespot, but hydras and
worms also have light/dark sensitivity cells located in their outer epidermal
layers. Eventually, a pinhole eyespot formed, which enabled even more
complex processing of visual information. Of course, in order for this
eyespot to have occurred, other parts of a rudimentary nervous system had
to be in place, like the mass of neurons localized in one area, such as the
head.
For example, the nautilus—one of the so-called living fossils, because it
morphologically has not changed much since the Cambrian explosion—is
representative of this next step in the evolution of the eye. The evolution
of the visual system likely began with a general light sensitivity around the ventricle of the forebrain. These forebrain neurons were transformed
in the course of evolution into an assemblage of neurons capable of analyzing
strictly visual information, rather than other information carried to
this area by other input neurons. The processed visual information was
transferred through relay neurons (the future ganglion cells) to the contralateral
part of the brain. In subsequent generations, this primordial
retina evaginated (formed a cup-like hole) and eventually transformed into
the lateral eyes of vertebrates (Cronly-Dillon, 1991).
Once this rudimentary eye developed, it took different directions in
terms of its formation among the various species that have vision. There
are all types of eyes falling into two broad categories: the compound eyes
of insects, crabs, and arthropods that are composed of hundreds of cylindrical
elements consisting of a lens, eight photoreceptor cells, and a sleeve
of opaque pigment cells, and the vertebrate eyes of fi sh, reptiles, and
mammals that all build upon the basic fl atworm eye having a retina, ganglion
cells, and projections to a brain. This divergence further ratifi es the
evolutionary principles of genetic variability and the natural selection of
the fi ttest traits given a specifi c environment. The kind of eye that fortuitously
developed, and then fl ourished, in the sand shark population under
the sea will be different from the eye that fortuitously developed, and then
fl ourished, in the black widow spider population on land, and so on.
As noted already, through investigation of the fossil endocasts of insectivores,
Jerison (1973, 1991) has offered a convincing case that the ancestor
of the primate was a small, nocturnal creature that prowled through trees
and bushes on the lookout for insects, as some small primates do today.
The eyes of this creature were close set and forward facing, indicating that
it was a predator. Predators like owls, cats, and primates have close set,
forward facing eyes with stereotopic vision, enabling them to more easily
identify depths and distances while hunting their prey. Animals like rabbits
and squirrels—the prey of predators like owls, cats, and some primates—
have eyes on the sides of their heads with panoramic vision, enabling them
to more easily identify predators from a variety of angles. The eyes of this
insectivore would have had an abundance of rods and fewer cones to
enable maneuverability in the dark. Insectivores had to forage on the
ground and in the dark in order to steer clear of bigger reptiles, amphibians,
and dinosaurs that would have preyed on them during that time.
However, when most of the dinosaurs and other larger predators were
wiped out by an asteroid at the close of the Cretaceous, this allowed for
the possibility of insectivores to occupy different niches. Eventually, some
of the descendents of these creatures did their foraging during the day,
The Evolution of the Visual System and Scenario Visualization 107
while others took to life in the trees. Still others, like our primate ancestors,
did both. By 20 mya, Proconsul had taken to life in the trees of Africa, most
likely because such a life off of the ground made it easier to avoid predators
lurking on the ground, as well as pounce upon prey that happened to be
on the ground. This life in the trees is evidenced by their money-like and
ape-like skeletons as well as their diet, which indicate a lifestyle not unlike
that of present-day monkeys and apes (Groves, 2002; Hartwig, 2002;
McGrew, 2004; Byrne & Whiten, 1988; Allman, 1982, 2000).
Life during the daytime and in the trees meant that a visual system, at
the very least, would need to have evolved more cones, the capacity to
distinguish colors, more memory-storage capacities, and facial recognition.
More cones would be needed to be able to handle the brighter light of
daytime living. A capacity to distinguish colors would be needed to determine
what foods one could eat, as well as distinguish foreground from
background while maneuvering through the trees. Rudimentary forms of
long-term and short-term memory would be needed for a mental map of
the area where this creature lived, as well as for distinguishing friend from
foe. Finally, facial recognition would be needed to distinguish friend from
foe, as well as to communicate various messages like “Leave me alone,”
“Back off,” “It’s o.k. to touch me,” and the like to other members of one’s
social group or to enemies. On top of all of this, arboreal life required
extremities more fi t for grasping, jumping, launching, and other sorts of
bodily maneuvering (Barton, 1998; Fleagle, 1999; Byrne, 1995, 2001; Rilling
& Insel, 1998; Allman, 1977).
In order to do all of this, the connections between eye and brain, as well
as those connections between other sensorimotor systems and brain, would
need to be more complex. As Jerison (1973, 1997) and Kaas (1996) point
out, the visual system of this early arboreal ancestor would have had a
brain very similar to that of a lemur or a monkey. This being the case, at
least three features of the brain and visual system would have needed to
be in place in Proconsul.
First, referring back to MacLean’s model, the reptilian, the paleomammalian,
and part of the neomammalian brain would have to be present in
Proconsul, since the amount of processing that such arboreal ancestors had
to do required a brain larger than that of reptiles. The amount of processing
that the visual system does alone would constitute a bigger brain.
However, arboreal social life entailed the interaction of sensory modalities
in combination with memory, emotion, planning, and motor function. All
of this processing required a bigger brain, more synaptic connections, and
more types of neural connections. In addition, the system must have been
a hierarchical organization of processes and subsystems so that the right
data could be selected and information could be integrated as Proconsul
went about the business of negotiating its environment.
Second, in order to organize and coordinate information from the specialized
visual modules and each of the other sensory modalities, integrative
or association areas and systems had to evolve. Research by Kaas (1987,
1993, 1995, 1996), Bear et al. (2001), Uylings & van Eden (1990), Roberts,
Robbins, & Weiskrantz (1998), and Hofman (2001) has shown that it is
primarily the association areas of the brain that increase in the evolution
of the mammal. The association areas show a marked increase when we
compare the brains of rats, cats, macaque monkeys, and humans, in that
order. On inspection, the visual, auditory, and sensorimotor areas of a rat
basically butt up against one another with very little of the cortex—some
15%–20%—devoted to the association of these areas. Some 40%–50% of
the frontal, parietal, and temporal lobes of the macaque monkey are
devoted to association of sensorimotor information. Even more cortex—
some 50%–60%—is devoted to association of sensorimotor information in
humans.
Third, the visual areas would have needed to be linked up with the motor
areas, so that visual information could be communicated to the brain and
then translated into voluntary motion. The motor areas of primates receive
inputs from the thalamic nuclei that relay information from the basal telencephalon
and the cerebellum, and they send outputs to motor control
neurons in the brain and spinal cord. The motor area is situated next to
the somatosensory area, and both are located in the frontal lobe near the
central sulcus at the parietofrontal junction of the brain. The sensorimotor
area—along with the auditory and visual areas—send projections to association
areas in the parietal and temporal cortices. From there, the projections
are sent to the multimodal areas located in the parietotemporal,
prefrontal, and limbic cortices (Kandel et al., 2000). These connections
would have had to be in place in the brain of Proconsul so that it could
have acted on visual information it received.
In the last chapter, I mentioned the idea put forward by the Gestalt
theorists and William James that the mind and the world are “something
of a mutual fi t.” This idea should be kept in mind here as we investigate
the evolution of visual modules, along with the evolution of other sensory
modules and mechanisms of selectivity and integration. Events in the
physical environment are composed of materials that the human brain,
complete with its specialized modules, has evolved to perceive and discriminate.
These materials take forms ranging from specifi c chemicals, to
The Evolution of the Visual System and Scenario Visualization 109
mechanical energy, to electromagnetic radiation (light) and are discriminated
by the different sensory modalities that are specifi cally attuned to
these stimuli: taste and smell are specialized for processing concentrations
of different chemicals, touch is specialized for processing mechanical
deformations of the skin, hearing is specialized for the intake of sound
waves, and vision is specialized for the intake of electromagnetic radiation.
Thus, given the environment surrounding an animal, it makes sense that
the CNS is a hierarchically organized system composed of specialized
modules, as well as mechanisms of selectivity and integration, in order to
process the myriad pieces of environmental information.