4.5 Tools and the Visual System
К оглавлению1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 1617 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33
34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41
The hominin line diverged from the common ancestor of African apes
some 6 mya. As was noted above, the brains of Australopithecines got no
bigger than that of a chimpanzee. Therefore, from research on primates
and monkeys, coupled with the archeological and geological data, we have
an idea of what the brain and visual system of Australopithecines may have
looked like and how they would have performed (Dunbar, 1988; see the
articles in Berthelet & Chavaillon, 1993). The neurophysiology and lifestyle
of Early Australopithecines, like A. anamensis, were probably similar to
those of present-day chimps.
However, the climate in Africa slowly changed. Approximately 8 mya,
present-day India (which was separate from Asia prior to this time) butted
up against Asia and formed the Himalayan mountains, thereby robbing
Africa of excess amounts of water. This climatic change caused eastern
Africa to be transformed from predominantly forested environments, to
savanna-like environments of forest and open range, to open range environments
with little or no forestation (Calvin, 2001, 2004; Potts, 1996;
Eldredge, 2001). Daly & Wilson (1999), Foley (1995), and Boyd & Silk
(1997) have shown that the Pleistocene epoch did not consist of a single
hunter–gatherer type of environment but was actually a constellation of
environments that presented a host of challenges to the early hominin
mind. The climate shift in Africa from jungle life to desert/savanna life
forced our early hominins to come out of the trees and survive in totally
new environments (Tattersall, 2001; Calvin, 2004; Aiello, 1997; Fleagle,
1999).
If the Australopithecines wanted to continue to live a life in the trees, they
would have had to spend more time on the ground moving from tree to
tree, so they must have had to utilize all four of their extremities more often. As Aiello (1997) notes, knuckle-walking and knuckle-running are
quite taxing on a body. Ultimately, mutations coded for a semi-erect
posture, and other mutations coded for an erect posture. Such fortuitous
mutations would have been fi tting for a savanna lifestyle where one could
be more effi cient at foraging, scavenging, and running away from predators
if one were on two legs, freeing up the arms for other activities.
Along with bipedalism, it is generally agreed by biologists, anthropologists,
archeologists, and other researchers that a variety of factors contributed
to the evolution of the human brain. These factors include, but are
by no means limited to, such things as diversifi ed habitats, social systems,
protein from large animals, higher amounts of starch, delayed consumption
of food, food sharing, language, and toolmaking (Aboitiz, 1996; Aiello,
1996, 1997; Aiello & Dunbar, 1993; Allman, 2000; Byrne, 1995; Deacon,
1997; Donald, 1991, 1997; Gibson & Ingold, 1993; Lock, 1993; Calvin,
1998, 2001, 2004; Dawkins, 2005). It is not possible to get a complete
picture of the evolution of the brain without looking at all of these factors,
as brain development is involved in a complex coevolution with physiology,
environment, and social circumstances. And, most obviously, the
emergence of language in our species occupies a central place with respect
to our ability to fl ourish and dominate the earth (Tallerman, 2005; Christiansen
& Kirby, 2003; Deacon, 1997; Mithen, 1996; Aiello & Dunbar,
1993). However, I wish to focus on toolmaking as essential in the evolution
of the brain and visual system. I do this for three reasons.
First, toolmaking is the mark of intelligence that distinguishes the Australopithecine
species from the Homo species in our evolutionary past. Homo
habilis was the fi rst toolmaker, as the Latin name (handy-man) suggests.
Second, tools offer us indirect—but compelling—evidence that psychological
states emerged from brain states. In trying to simulate ancient toolmaking
techniques, archeologists have discovered that certain tools only can
be made according to mental templates, goal formations, and scenarios, as
Pelegrin (1993), Isaac (1986), and Wynn (1979, 1981, 1991, 1993) have
demonstrated. Finally, as I show, the evolution of toolmaking parallels the
evolution of the visual system from noncognitive visual processing to
conscious cognitive visual processing in terms of scenario visualization. Let
me explicate these three points.
As was noted above, Proconsul most likely had a brain similar to that of
a monkey. The Australopithecine brain was not much larger but probably
had all of the same neural connections as that of a present-day chimpanzee.
Thus, we can look to the chimpanzee and its usage of tools as an
indicator of what Australopithecine tool usage may have been like. Chimps
The Evolution of the Visual System and Scenario Visualization 111
can strip leaves from branches and use these branches to fi sh for termites,
knock fruit from a tree, or hit other chimps. They also use leaves to construct
little makeshift baskets to carry food, protect themselves from rain,
or scoop up water to drink. Further, they use stones to crack open nuts
and other husks in order to extract food (McGrew, 2004; Byrne, 1995,
2001; Byrne & Whiten, 1988; Griffi n, 1992; Stanford, 2000). It is likely that
the Australopithecines did similar things.
The fi rst stone artifacts, Oldowan stones, date to between 3 and 2 mya
and usually are associated with Homo habilis. They consist of fl akes, choppers,
and bone breakers that likely were used to break open the bones of
animals in order to get at the protein-rich marrow (Pelegrin, 1993; Isaac,
1986; Wynn, 1981; Wynn & McGrew, 1989; Mithen, 1996). Most probably,
Homo habilis had to wait until other bigger animals killed their prey and
feasted before they went in and scavenged what was left of the carcass.
The key innovation has to do with the technique of chopping stones to
create a chopping or cutting edge. Typically, many fl akes and the like were
struck from a single core stone, using a softer hammer stone to strike the
blow.
Here, we have the fi rst instance of making a tool to make another tool,
and it is arguable that this technique is what distinguishes ape-men from
apes. Another way to put this is that chimps and Australopithecines used
the tools they made but did not use these tools they made to make other
tools. Thus, the distinction is between a tool user who has made a certain
tool A to serve some function (e.g., Australopithecines and chimps) and a
toolmaker who has made a certain tool A to serve the specifi c function of
making another tool B to serve some function (e.g., Homo habilis).
Also, as has been noted, the brain of Homo habilis was approximately
700 cm in volume, almost twice the size of the Australopithecine’s brain. It
is likely that this increase in brain size contributed to the move from tool
usage to toolmaking. What this means is that, through the combined
factors of genetic mutation and environmental pressures, the brain evolved
the capacity for more complex connections and capacities, setting up the
conditions for the possibility of toolmaking. Those hominins who were either
lucky enough or resourceful enough to discover the benefi ts of toolmaking
began to do so and survived to reproduce more “crafty” hominins like
themselves.
The Acheulian tool industry consisted of axes, picks, and cleavers. It fi rst
appeared around 1.5 mya and usually is associated with Homo erectus or
Homo ergaster. The key innovations include the shaping of an entire stone
to a stereotyped tool form, as well as producing a symmetrical (bifacial) cutting edge by chipping the stone from both sides. It seems that the same
tools were being used for a variety of tasks such as slicing open animal
skins, carving meat, and breaking bones (Pelegrin, 1993; Isaac, 1986;
Mithen, 1996; Wynn, 1979). Consistent with the increase in the complexity
of toolmaking, the brain of Homo erectus increased to 900 cm in volume,
up 200 cm from Homo habilis. The Oldowan and Acheulian industries are
part of the time period commonly referred to as the Lower Paleolithic.
The Acheulian industry stayed in place for over a million years. The next
breakthrough in tool technology was the Mousterian industry that arrived
on the scene with the Homo neandertalensis lineage, near the end of the
Homo heidelbergensis lineage, around 300,000 ya. Mousterian techniques
(also called Levallois methods) involved a more complex three-stage process
of constructing (1) the basic core stone, (2) the rough blank, and (3) the
refi ned fi nalized tool. Such a process enabled various kinds of tools to be
created, since the rough blank could follow a pattern that ultimately would
become fl ake blades, scrapers, cutting tools, serrated tools, or lances.
Further, these tools had wider applications as they were being used with
other material components to form handles and spears, and they were
being used as tools to make other tools, such as wooden and bone artifacts
(Pelegrin, 1993; Isaac, 1986; Mithen, 1996; Wynn, 1981, 1991). Again,
consistent with the increase in complexity of toolmaking, the brain of
Homo heidelbergensis and Homo neandertalensis increased to 1,200 cm and
1,500 cm in volume, respectively, up 300 to 600 cm from Homo erectus. The
Mousterian industry is part of the time period commonly referred to as the
Middle Paleolithic.
By 40,000 ya, some 80,000 years after anatomically modern Homo sapiens
evolved, we fi nd instances of human art in the forms of beads, tooth necklaces,
cave paintings, stone carvings, and fi gurines. This period in tool
manufacture is known as the Upper Paleolithic industry, and it ranges from
40,000 ya to the advent of agriculture around 12,000 ya. Sewing needles
and fi sh hooks made of bone and antlers fi rst appear, along with fl aked
stones for arrows and spears, burins (chisel-like stones for working bone
and ivory), multibarbed harpoon points, and spear throwers made of
wood, bone, or antler (Pelegrin, 1993; Isaac, 1986; Mithen, 1996; Wynn,
1991). The human brain during this period was about the same size as it
is today, between 1,200 and 1,400 cm in volume.
In the previous chapter, I distinguished four levels of visual processing.
The fi rst level is a noncognitive visual processing that occurs at the lowest
level of the visual hierarchy associated with the eye, LGN, and primary
visual cortex. The second level of visual processing is a cognitive visual
The Evolution of the Visual System and Scenario Visualization 113
processing that occurs at a higher level of visual awareness associated with
the what and where visual unimodal areas. As I noted, the move from
noncognitive visual processing to cognitive visual processing is a move
from the purely neurobiological to the psychoneurobiological dimension
of the brain. The third level of visual processing is a cognitive visual processing
that consists of the integration of visual information in the visual
unimodal association area. The fourth level of visual processing is a conscious
cognitive visual processing that occurs at the highest level of the
visual hierarchy concerned with the multimodal areas, frontal areas, and,
most probably, the summated areas of the cerebral cortex.
The question now becomes this: What level of visual processing did these
early hominins achieve in light of the size of their brains and the tools
they constructed? Given the neural connections of present-day chimps, as
well as the eye-socket formations and endocasts of Australopithecines, it is
clear that both have (had) noncognitive visual processing. Also, given that
chimps are aware of and attend to visual stimuli, it is likely that they, along
with Australopithecines, have (had) cognitive visual processing. But can
these species be said to have conscious cognitive visual processing? Possibly,
to a certain extent. However, it is arguable that what counts against such
species having full conscious cognitive visual processing is a lack of
advanced forms of toolmaking, like those found in the Upper Paleolithic
industry. There seems to be a direct connection between advanced forms
of toolmaking and conscious visual processing. What exactly is that
connection?
The hominin line diverged from the common ancestor of African apes
some 6 mya. As was noted above, the brains of Australopithecines got no
bigger than that of a chimpanzee. Therefore, from research on primates
and monkeys, coupled with the archeological and geological data, we have
an idea of what the brain and visual system of Australopithecines may have
looked like and how they would have performed (Dunbar, 1988; see the
articles in Berthelet & Chavaillon, 1993). The neurophysiology and lifestyle
of Early Australopithecines, like A. anamensis, were probably similar to
those of present-day chimps.
However, the climate in Africa slowly changed. Approximately 8 mya,
present-day India (which was separate from Asia prior to this time) butted
up against Asia and formed the Himalayan mountains, thereby robbing
Africa of excess amounts of water. This climatic change caused eastern
Africa to be transformed from predominantly forested environments, to
savanna-like environments of forest and open range, to open range environments
with little or no forestation (Calvin, 2001, 2004; Potts, 1996;
Eldredge, 2001). Daly & Wilson (1999), Foley (1995), and Boyd & Silk
(1997) have shown that the Pleistocene epoch did not consist of a single
hunter–gatherer type of environment but was actually a constellation of
environments that presented a host of challenges to the early hominin
mind. The climate shift in Africa from jungle life to desert/savanna life
forced our early hominins to come out of the trees and survive in totally
new environments (Tattersall, 2001; Calvin, 2004; Aiello, 1997; Fleagle,
1999).
If the Australopithecines wanted to continue to live a life in the trees, they
would have had to spend more time on the ground moving from tree to
tree, so they must have had to utilize all four of their extremities more often. As Aiello (1997) notes, knuckle-walking and knuckle-running are
quite taxing on a body. Ultimately, mutations coded for a semi-erect
posture, and other mutations coded for an erect posture. Such fortuitous
mutations would have been fi tting for a savanna lifestyle where one could
be more effi cient at foraging, scavenging, and running away from predators
if one were on two legs, freeing up the arms for other activities.
Along with bipedalism, it is generally agreed by biologists, anthropologists,
archeologists, and other researchers that a variety of factors contributed
to the evolution of the human brain. These factors include, but are
by no means limited to, such things as diversifi ed habitats, social systems,
protein from large animals, higher amounts of starch, delayed consumption
of food, food sharing, language, and toolmaking (Aboitiz, 1996; Aiello,
1996, 1997; Aiello & Dunbar, 1993; Allman, 2000; Byrne, 1995; Deacon,
1997; Donald, 1991, 1997; Gibson & Ingold, 1993; Lock, 1993; Calvin,
1998, 2001, 2004; Dawkins, 2005). It is not possible to get a complete
picture of the evolution of the brain without looking at all of these factors,
as brain development is involved in a complex coevolution with physiology,
environment, and social circumstances. And, most obviously, the
emergence of language in our species occupies a central place with respect
to our ability to fl ourish and dominate the earth (Tallerman, 2005; Christiansen
& Kirby, 2003; Deacon, 1997; Mithen, 1996; Aiello & Dunbar,
1993). However, I wish to focus on toolmaking as essential in the evolution
of the brain and visual system. I do this for three reasons.
First, toolmaking is the mark of intelligence that distinguishes the Australopithecine
species from the Homo species in our evolutionary past. Homo
habilis was the fi rst toolmaker, as the Latin name (handy-man) suggests.
Second, tools offer us indirect—but compelling—evidence that psychological
states emerged from brain states. In trying to simulate ancient toolmaking
techniques, archeologists have discovered that certain tools only can
be made according to mental templates, goal formations, and scenarios, as
Pelegrin (1993), Isaac (1986), and Wynn (1979, 1981, 1991, 1993) have
demonstrated. Finally, as I show, the evolution of toolmaking parallels the
evolution of the visual system from noncognitive visual processing to
conscious cognitive visual processing in terms of scenario visualization. Let
me explicate these three points.
As was noted above, Proconsul most likely had a brain similar to that of
a monkey. The Australopithecine brain was not much larger but probably
had all of the same neural connections as that of a present-day chimpanzee.
Thus, we can look to the chimpanzee and its usage of tools as an
indicator of what Australopithecine tool usage may have been like. Chimps
The Evolution of the Visual System and Scenario Visualization 111
can strip leaves from branches and use these branches to fi sh for termites,
knock fruit from a tree, or hit other chimps. They also use leaves to construct
little makeshift baskets to carry food, protect themselves from rain,
or scoop up water to drink. Further, they use stones to crack open nuts
and other husks in order to extract food (McGrew, 2004; Byrne, 1995,
2001; Byrne & Whiten, 1988; Griffi n, 1992; Stanford, 2000). It is likely that
the Australopithecines did similar things.
The fi rst stone artifacts, Oldowan stones, date to between 3 and 2 mya
and usually are associated with Homo habilis. They consist of fl akes, choppers,
and bone breakers that likely were used to break open the bones of
animals in order to get at the protein-rich marrow (Pelegrin, 1993; Isaac,
1986; Wynn, 1981; Wynn & McGrew, 1989; Mithen, 1996). Most probably,
Homo habilis had to wait until other bigger animals killed their prey and
feasted before they went in and scavenged what was left of the carcass.
The key innovation has to do with the technique of chopping stones to
create a chopping or cutting edge. Typically, many fl akes and the like were
struck from a single core stone, using a softer hammer stone to strike the
blow.
Here, we have the fi rst instance of making a tool to make another tool,
and it is arguable that this technique is what distinguishes ape-men from
apes. Another way to put this is that chimps and Australopithecines used
the tools they made but did not use these tools they made to make other
tools. Thus, the distinction is between a tool user who has made a certain
tool A to serve some function (e.g., Australopithecines and chimps) and a
toolmaker who has made a certain tool A to serve the specifi c function of
making another tool B to serve some function (e.g., Homo habilis).
Also, as has been noted, the brain of Homo habilis was approximately
700 cm in volume, almost twice the size of the Australopithecine’s brain. It
is likely that this increase in brain size contributed to the move from tool
usage to toolmaking. What this means is that, through the combined
factors of genetic mutation and environmental pressures, the brain evolved
the capacity for more complex connections and capacities, setting up the
conditions for the possibility of toolmaking. Those hominins who were either
lucky enough or resourceful enough to discover the benefi ts of toolmaking
began to do so and survived to reproduce more “crafty” hominins like
themselves.
The Acheulian tool industry consisted of axes, picks, and cleavers. It fi rst
appeared around 1.5 mya and usually is associated with Homo erectus or
Homo ergaster. The key innovations include the shaping of an entire stone
to a stereotyped tool form, as well as producing a symmetrical (bifacial) cutting edge by chipping the stone from both sides. It seems that the same
tools were being used for a variety of tasks such as slicing open animal
skins, carving meat, and breaking bones (Pelegrin, 1993; Isaac, 1986;
Mithen, 1996; Wynn, 1979). Consistent with the increase in the complexity
of toolmaking, the brain of Homo erectus increased to 900 cm in volume,
up 200 cm from Homo habilis. The Oldowan and Acheulian industries are
part of the time period commonly referred to as the Lower Paleolithic.
The Acheulian industry stayed in place for over a million years. The next
breakthrough in tool technology was the Mousterian industry that arrived
on the scene with the Homo neandertalensis lineage, near the end of the
Homo heidelbergensis lineage, around 300,000 ya. Mousterian techniques
(also called Levallois methods) involved a more complex three-stage process
of constructing (1) the basic core stone, (2) the rough blank, and (3) the
refi ned fi nalized tool. Such a process enabled various kinds of tools to be
created, since the rough blank could follow a pattern that ultimately would
become fl ake blades, scrapers, cutting tools, serrated tools, or lances.
Further, these tools had wider applications as they were being used with
other material components to form handles and spears, and they were
being used as tools to make other tools, such as wooden and bone artifacts
(Pelegrin, 1993; Isaac, 1986; Mithen, 1996; Wynn, 1981, 1991). Again,
consistent with the increase in complexity of toolmaking, the brain of
Homo heidelbergensis and Homo neandertalensis increased to 1,200 cm and
1,500 cm in volume, respectively, up 300 to 600 cm from Homo erectus. The
Mousterian industry is part of the time period commonly referred to as the
Middle Paleolithic.
By 40,000 ya, some 80,000 years after anatomically modern Homo sapiens
evolved, we fi nd instances of human art in the forms of beads, tooth necklaces,
cave paintings, stone carvings, and fi gurines. This period in tool
manufacture is known as the Upper Paleolithic industry, and it ranges from
40,000 ya to the advent of agriculture around 12,000 ya. Sewing needles
and fi sh hooks made of bone and antlers fi rst appear, along with fl aked
stones for arrows and spears, burins (chisel-like stones for working bone
and ivory), multibarbed harpoon points, and spear throwers made of
wood, bone, or antler (Pelegrin, 1993; Isaac, 1986; Mithen, 1996; Wynn,
1991). The human brain during this period was about the same size as it
is today, between 1,200 and 1,400 cm in volume.
In the previous chapter, I distinguished four levels of visual processing.
The fi rst level is a noncognitive visual processing that occurs at the lowest
level of the visual hierarchy associated with the eye, LGN, and primary
visual cortex. The second level of visual processing is a cognitive visual
The Evolution of the Visual System and Scenario Visualization 113
processing that occurs at a higher level of visual awareness associated with
the what and where visual unimodal areas. As I noted, the move from
noncognitive visual processing to cognitive visual processing is a move
from the purely neurobiological to the psychoneurobiological dimension
of the brain. The third level of visual processing is a cognitive visual processing
that consists of the integration of visual information in the visual
unimodal association area. The fourth level of visual processing is a conscious
cognitive visual processing that occurs at the highest level of the
visual hierarchy concerned with the multimodal areas, frontal areas, and,
most probably, the summated areas of the cerebral cortex.
The question now becomes this: What level of visual processing did these
early hominins achieve in light of the size of their brains and the tools
they constructed? Given the neural connections of present-day chimps, as
well as the eye-socket formations and endocasts of Australopithecines, it is
clear that both have (had) noncognitive visual processing. Also, given that
chimps are aware of and attend to visual stimuli, it is likely that they, along
with Australopithecines, have (had) cognitive visual processing. But can
these species be said to have conscious cognitive visual processing? Possibly,
to a certain extent. However, it is arguable that what counts against such
species having full conscious cognitive visual processing is a lack of
advanced forms of toolmaking, like those found in the Upper Paleolithic
industry. There seems to be a direct connection between advanced forms
of toolmaking and conscious visual processing. What exactly is that
connection?